Preferencias por estímulos sexuales eróticos según el género y la orientación sexual: estudio de rastreo ocular
dc.contributor.advisor | Vásquez Amézquita, Milena | |
dc.contributor.author | Valenzuela Arias, Sebastián Camilo | |
dc.contributor.author | Rivero Valderrama, Ángela | |
dc.date.accessioned | 2024-11-07T19:54:42Z | |
dc.date.available | 2024-11-07T19:54:42Z | |
dc.date.issued | 2024-10 | |
dc.description.abstract | Los hombres y las mujeres procesan diferencialmente los estímulos sexuales según su orientación sexual y género. Estos estímulos sexuales desencadenan respuestas hormonales, emocionales y conductuales a través del procesamiento automático y el procesamiento consciente de señales sexuales que inherentemente afectan la atención visual y la experiencia de excitación sexual. No es claro cómo difiere el patrón de atención visual hacia estímulos eróticos en competencia con estímulos no eróticos y sí este patrón de respuesta es específico de género y consistente con la orientación sexual en hombres y mujeres heterosexuales, bisexuales y homosexuales. Este estudio exploró las preferencias por estímulos sexuales eróticos según el género y la orientación sexual a través del patrón de atención visual y la evaluación subjetiva de la excitación sexual de 116 hombres (70 heterosexuales, 37 homosexuales y 9 bisexuales) y 103 mujeres (59 heterosexuales, 20 homosexuales y 24 bisexuales) con edades promedio de 22 años (DE=4.00) por medio de la técnica de rastreo ocular. Se encontró un patrón de interés visual específico de género y según la orientación sexual sobre el cuerpo entero, pecho y los genitales (pelvis) diferenciado para estímulos eróticos, comparado con estímulos no eróticos. Se concluye que el género, y además la orientación sexual predicen significativamente la atención controlada hacia estímulos eróticos del género preferido, con variaciones en la excitación sexual subjetiva solo entre mujeres heterosexuales y homosexuales. La técnica de rastreo ocular puede ser una herramienta útil para medir fiablemente el interés sexual hacia estímulos eróticos. | |
dc.description.abstractenglish | Men and women differentially process sexual stimuli according to their gender. These sexual stimuli trigger hormonal, emotional and behavioral responses through automatic processing and conscious processing of sexual cues that inherently affect visual attention and the experience of sexual arousal. It is unclear how the pattern of visual attention to erotic stimuli differs in competition with non-erotic stimuli and whether this pattern of response is gender-specific and consistent with sexual orientation in heterosexual, bisexual, and homosexual men and women. This study explored preferences for erotic sexual stimuli by gender and sexual orientation through the pattern of visual attention and subjective assessment of sexual arousal of 116 men (70 heterosexual, 37 homosexual and 9 bisexual) and 103 women (59 heterosexual, 20 homosexual and 24 bisexual) with mean ages of 22 years (SD=4.00) by means of the eye- tracking technique. A gender-specific and sexual orientation-specific pattern of visual interest in the whole body, breast and genitalia (pelvis) was found for erotic stimuli, compared to non- erotic stimuli. It is concluded that gender, and furthermore sexual orientation significantly predict controlled attention to erotic stimuli of the preferred gender, with variations in subjective sexual arousal. The eye-tracking technique may be a reliable tool for measuring sexual interest in erotic stimuli. | |
dc.description.degreelevel | Pregrado | spa |
dc.description.degreename | Psicólogo | spa |
dc.format.mimetype | application/pdf | |
dc.identifier.instname | instname:Universidad El Bosque | spa |
dc.identifier.reponame | reponame:Repositorio Institucional Universidad El Bosque | spa |
dc.identifier.repourl | repourl:https://repositorio.unbosque.edu.co | |
dc.identifier.uri | https://hdl.handle.net/20.500.12495/13145 | |
dc.language.iso | es | |
dc.publisher.faculty | Facultad de Psicología | spa |
dc.publisher.grantor | Universidad El Bosque | spa |
dc.publisher.program | Psicología | spa |
dc.relation.references | Basson, R. (2002). A Model of Women’s Sexual Arousal. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy, 28(1), 1-10. https://doi.org/10.1080/009262302317250963 | |
dc.relation.references | Baumeister, R. F. (2000). Gender differences in erotic plasticity: The female sex drive as socially flexible and responsive. Psychological Bulletin, 126(3), 347-374. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-2909.126.3.347 | |
dc.relation.references | Bränström, R., Pachankis, J., Jin, J., Klein, D., & Hatzenbuehler, M. (2023). Self-Schemas and Information Processing Biases as Mechanisms Underlying Sexual Orientation Disparities in Depressive Symptoms: Results From a Longitudinal, Population-Based Study. Journal of Psychopathology and Clinical Science, 132(6), 681-693. https://doi.org/10.1037/abn0000823 | |
dc.relation.references | Calvo, M. G., & Lang, P. J. (2004). Gaze Patterns When Looking at Emotional Pictures: Motivationally Biased Attention. Motivation and Emotion, 28(3), 221-243. https://doi.org/10.1023/B:MOEM.0000040153.26156.ed | |
dc.relation.references | Carretié, L. (2014). Exogenous (automatic) attention to emotional stimuli: A review. Cognitive, Affective & Behavioral Neuroscience, 14(4), 1228-1258. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13415-014-0270-2 | |
dc.relation.references | Chivers, M. L. (2017). The Specificity of Women’s Sexual Response and Its Relationship with Sexual Orientations: A Review and Ten Hypotheses. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 46(5), 1161-1179. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-016-0897-x | |
dc.relation.references | Chivers, M. L., & Bailey, J. M. (2005). A sex difference in features that elicit genital response. Biological Psychology, 70(2), 115-120. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2004.12.002 | |
dc.relation.references | Dawson, S. J., & Chivers, M. L. (2018). The effect of static versus dynamic stimuli on visual processing of sexual cues in androphilic women and gynephilic men. Royal Society Open Science, 5(6), 172286. https://doi.org/10.1098/rsos.172286 | |
dc.relation.references | Feng, C., Wang, L., Wang, N., Gu, R., & Luo, Y. (2012). The time course of implicit processing of erotic pictures: An event-related potential study. Brain Research, 1489, 48-55. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.brainres.2012.10.019 | |
dc.relation.references | Godet, T., & Niveau, G. (2021). Eye tracking and child sexual offenders: A systematic review. Forensic Sciences Research, 6(2), 133-140. https://doi.org/10.1080/20961790.2021.1940737 | |
dc.relation.references | Janssen, E., Everaerd, W., Spiering, M., & Janssen, J. (2000). Automatic processes and the appraisal of sexual stimuli: Toward an information processing model of sexual arousal. The Journal of Sex Research, 37(1), 8-23. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224490009552016 | |
dc.relation.references | Lykins, A. D., Meana, M., & Kambe, G. (2006). Detection of Differential Viewing Patterns to Erotic and Non-Erotic Stimuli Using Eye-Tracking Methodology. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 35(5), 569-575. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-006-9065-z | |
dc.relation.references | Lykins, A. D., Meana, M., & Strauss, G. P. (2008). Sex Differences in Visual Attention to Erotic and Non-Erotic Stimuli. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 37(2), 219-228. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-007-9208-x | |
dc.relation.references | Morandini, J. S., Spence, B., Dar-Nimrod, I., & Lykins, A. D. (2020). Do Bisexuals Have a Bisexual Viewing Pattern? Archives of Sexual Behavior, 49(2), 489-502. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-019-01514-y | |
dc.relation.references | Navarro, D. J., & Foxcroft, D. R. (2018). Learning statistics with jamovi: A tutorial for psychology students and other beginners. Danielle J. Navarro and David R. Foxcroft. https://doi.org/10.24384/HGC3-7P15 | |
dc.relation.references | Pessoa, L., Kastner, S., & Ungerleider, L. G. (2002). Attentional control of the processing of neutral and emotional stimuli. Cognitive Brain Research, 15(1), 31-45. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0926-6410(02)00214-8 | |
dc.relation.references | Ponseti, J., Bosinski, H. A., Wolff, S., Peller, M., Jansen, O., Mehdorn, H. M., Büchel, C., & Siebner, H. R. (2006). A functional endophenotype for sexual orientation in humans. NeuroImage, 33(3), 825-833. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.neuroimage.2006.08.002 | |
dc.relation.references | Rieger, G., Cash, B. M., Merrill, S. M., Jones-Rounds, J., Dharmavaram, S. M., & Savin- Williams, R. C. (2015). Sexual arousal: The correspondence of eyes and genitals. Biological Psychology, 104, 56-64. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.11.009 | |
dc.relation.references | Rupp, H. A., & Wallen, K. (2007). Sex differences in viewing sexual stimuli: An eye-tracking study in men and women. Hormones and Behavior, 51(4), 524-533. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.yhbeh.2007.01.008 | |
dc.relation.references | Safron, A., Sylva, D., Klimaj, V., Rosenthal, A. M., & Bailey, J. M. (2020). Neural Responses to Sexual Stimuli in Heterosexual and Homosexual Men and Women: Men’s Responses Are More Specific. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 49(2), 433-445. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10508-019-01521-z | |
dc.relation.references | Safron, A., Sylva, D., Klimaj, V., Rosenthal, A. M., Li, M., Walter, M., & Bailey, J. M. (2017). Neural Correlates of Sexual Orientation in Heterosexual, Bisexual, and Homosexual Men. Scientific Reports, 7(1), 41314. https://doi.org/10.1038/srep41314 | |
dc.relation.references | Sennwald, V., Pool, E., Brosch, T., Delplanque, S., Bianchi-Demicheli, F., & Sander, D. (2016). Emotional attention for erotic stimuli: Cognitive and brain mechanisms. Journal of Comparative Neurology, 524(8), 1668-1675. https://doi.org/10.1002/cne.23859 | |
dc.relation.references | Spape, J., Timmers, A. D., Yoon, S., Ponseti, J., & Chivers, M. L. (2014). Gender-specific genital and subjective sexual arousal to prepotent sexual features in heterosexual women and men. Biological Psychology, 102, 1-9. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biopsycho.2014.07.008 | |
dc.relation.references | Tassone, D., Dawson, S. J., & Chivers, M. L. (2019). The impact of homonegativity on gynephilic men’s visual attention toward non-preferred sexual targets. Personality and Individual Differences, 149, 261-272. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2019.05.062 | |
dc.relation.references | Theeuwes, J. (1991). Exogenous and endogenous control of attention: The effect of visual onsets and offsets. Perception & Psychophysics, 49(1), 83-90. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03211619 | |
dc.relation.references | Toates, F. (2009). An Integrative Theoretical Framework for Understanding Sexual Motivation, Arousal, and Behavior. The Journal of Sex Research, 46(2-3), 168-193. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224490902747768 | |
dc.relation.references | Vásquez Amézquita, M., Leongoméz, J. D., Seto, M. C., & Salvador, A. (2019). Differences in Visual Attention Patterns to Sexually Mature and Immature Stimuli Between Heterosexual Sexual Offenders, Nonsexual Offenders, and Nonoffending Men. The Journal of Sex Research, 56(2), 213-228. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2018.1511965 | |
dc.relation.references | Vásquez-Amézquita, M., Leongómez, J. D., Salvador, A., & Seto, M. C. (2023). What can the eyes tell us about atypical sexual preferences as a function of sex and age? Linking eye movements with child-related chronophilias. Forensic Sciences Research, 8(1), 5- 15. https://doi.org/10.1093/fsr/owad009 | |
dc.relation.references | Vásquez-Amézquita, M., Leongómez, J. D., Seto, M. C., Bonilla, M., Rodríguez-Padilla, A., & Salvador, A. (2017). Visual Attention Patterns Differ in Gynephilic and Androphilic Men and Women Depending on Age and Gender of Targets. The Journal of Sex Research, 56(1), 85-101. https://doi.org/10.1080/00224499.2017.1372353 | |
dc.rights | Atribución-NoComercial-CompartirIgual 4.0 Internacional | en |
dc.rights.accessrights | info:eu-repo/semantics/openAccess | |
dc.rights.accessrights | http:/purl.org/coar/access_right/c_abf2/ | |
dc.rights.local | Acceso abierto | spa |
dc.rights.uri | http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc-sa/4.0/ | |
dc.source.url | https://vimeo.com/1024047820 | |
dc.subject | Atención visual | |
dc.subject | Estímulos eróticos | |
dc.subject | Rastreo ocular | |
dc.subject | Especificidad de género | |
dc.subject | Orientación sexual | |
dc.subject.ddc | 150 | |
dc.subject.keywords | Visual attention | |
dc.subject.keywords | Erotic stimuli | |
dc.subject.keywords | Eye-tracking | |
dc.subject.keywords | Gender specificity | |
dc.subject.keywords | Sexual orientation | |
dc.title | Preferencias por estímulos sexuales eróticos según el género y la orientación sexual: estudio de rastreo ocular | |
dc.title.translated | Preferences for erotic sexual stimuli According to gender and sexual orientation: an eye-tracking study | |
dc.type.coar | https://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_7a1f | |
dc.type.coarversion | https://purl.org/coar/version/c_ab4af688f83e57aa | |
dc.type.driver | info:eu-repo/semantics/bachelorThesis | |
dc.type.hasversion | info:eu-repo/semantics/acceptedVersion | |
dc.type.local | Tesis/Trabajo de grado - Monografía - Pregrado | spa |
Archivos
Bloque de licencias
1 - 3 de 3
No hay miniatura disponible
- Nombre:
- license.txt
- Tamaño:
- 1.95 KB
- Formato:
- Item-specific license agreed upon to submission
- Descripción:
No hay miniatura disponible
- Nombre:
- Anexo 1 Acta de aprobacion.pdf
- Tamaño:
- 62.22 KB
- Formato:
- Adobe Portable Document Format
- Descripción:
No hay miniatura disponible
- Nombre:
- Carta de autorizacion.pdf
- Tamaño:
- 283.96 KB
- Formato:
- Adobe Portable Document Format
- Descripción: